How to Improve Your
Self-Control
"It's all right
letting yourself go, as long as you can get yourself back." ~Mick Jagger
New research suggests self-control can be improved using abstract
reasoning.
Temptation
comes in many forms, often so potent, so animal, that it seems
impossible to resist. Eating too much, drinking too much, spending too much or
letting the heart rule the head. We get instant messages from deep in the gut
that resonate through the mind, trying to dictate our behaviour.
One
of humanity's most useful skills, without which advanced civilisations would
not exist, is being able to engage our higher cognitive functions, our
self-control, to resist these temptations. Psychologists have found that
self-control is strongly associated with what we label success: higher
self-esteem, better interpersonal skills, better emotional responses and,
perhaps surprisingly, few drawbacks at even very high levels of self-control (Tangney et al., 2004).
People, being only
human, find the constant battle with basic urges is frequently too great and
their self-control buckles. However, recent experimental research by Dr
Kentaro Fujita at Ohio State
University and colleagues has explored ways of improving self-control, where it
comes from and why it sometimes deserts us.
Based on new research, along with studies conducted over the
past few decades, Dr Fujita and colleagues have proposed that abstract thinking
and psychological distance are particularly important in self-control.
1. Evidence that abstract thinking improves self-control
It never ceases to amaze just how different two people's views
of exactly the same event can be: one person's freedom fighter is another's
terrorist. But the way in which we view people or events isn't just constrained
by unchangeable patterns of thought that are set in stone. Dr Fujita and colleagues
explored the idea that simple manipulations of how we construe the world can
have a direct effect on self-control. Their hunch was that thinking from a more
abstract, high-level perspective increases self-control.
In their research,
published in the Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, Fujita et al. (2006) used
a number of experiments to test the idea that self-control is affected by how
we construe or interpret events. The problem for the researchers was
manipulating aspects of people's construal without them realising: this
required some deception.
In one of Fujita et al.'s (2006) studies participants were told
they were going to take part in two separate experiments - one on personality
and another billed as a student survey. In fact this was just a cover story as
the two pieces of research were designed to work together.
Experimenters used the 'student survey' as a cover to manipulate
levels of construal. They needed participants to be thinking in either a
high-level way (abstract - seeing the whole forest) or a low-level way
(concrete - seeing individual trees). They did this by getting participants to
think about their level of physical health, but in two different ways:
· High-level construal
condition: participants were asked to fill in
a diagram which encouraged them to think about why they maintain good physical health. Participants tended to
put answer such as: "To do well in school." This got them thinking
about ends rather than means - the ultimate purpose of physical health.
· Low-level construal
condition: in contrast participants in this
condition were asked to think about how they
maintained their physical health. Naturally they responded with things like:
"Go exercise". In other words they focused on means rather than ends,
the actual process.
Just before this manipulation of construal level, in a study
they were misinformed was separate, participants were told their personality
was being tested physiologically through holding a handgrip. This handgrip was
designed to be difficult to squeeze together but participants were told to hold
on as long as possible. This provided a baseline measurement of their grip
strength.
Just after the manipulation of construal level participants had
dummy electrodes attached to their arm and were told that their personality
could be measured while they squeezed the stiff handgrip again. This time,
though, they were told that the longer they could squeeze the handgrip the more
accurate the information would be. The question was: how well could
participants forget the temporary discomfort of holding the handgrip once they
had been told about the desired goal of getting information about their own
personalities?
The results confirmed
Fujita et al.'s (2006) suspicions. They showed that participants in the
low-construal thinking condition (thinking about means rather than ends) held
on to the handgrip for, on average, 4.9 seconds less than they had during the baseline measurement.
In contrast those in the
high-construal condition held on for 11.1 seconds longerthan their baseline measurement. Whether participants were
thinking about means or ends had a really significant effect on how long they
squeezed the handgrip. Those participants who had been encouraged to think in
high-level, abstract terms demonstrated greater self-control in enduring the
discomfort of the handgrip in order to receive more accurate personality
profiles.
Along with this design Fujita et al. (2006) also carried out
other studies using different measures of self-control and different ways of
inducing either high-level or low-level construal. These produced similar
findings. People in the high-level construal condition were consistently:
· More likely to avoid the temptation of instant gratification.
· Prepared to make a greater investment to learn more about their
health status.
· Less likely to evaluate temptations like beer and television
positively.
2. How personality and the situation affect self-control
Self-control is not just affected by how we are thinking at a
specific moment, that would be too easy. We have each developed different
amounts of self-control. Some people seem to find it easy to resist temptation
while others can be relied on to always yield to self-gratification. To a
certain extent we have to accept our starting point on the self-control sliding
scale and do the best we can with it.
Although a few people have very high (or very low) levels of
self-control, two-thirds of us lie somewhere near the middle: sometimes finding
it easy to resist temptation, other times not. Naturally the exact situation
has a huge effect on how much self-control we can exert. One property of
different situations central to self-control that psychologists have examined
is 'psychological distance'.
Research reveals that
people find it much easier to make decisions that demonstrate self-control when
they are thinking about events that are distant in time, for example how much
exercise they will do next week or what they will eat tomorrow (Fujita, 2008). Similarly they make much more disciplined decisions on behalf
of other people than they do for themselves. People implicitly follow the
maxim: do what I say, not what I do.
It's not hard to see the convergence between the idea of
'psychological distance' and high-level construal. Both emphasise the idea that
the more psychological or conceptual distance we can put between ourselves and
the particular decision or event, the more we are able to think about it in an
abstract way, and therefore the more self-control we can exert. It's all about
developing a special type of objectivity.
3. How to improve your self-control
Fujita et al.'s (2006)
studies, along with other similar findings reported by Fujita (2008), suggest that self-control can be increased by these related
ways of thinking:
· Global processing. This means trying to focus on the wood rather than the trees:
seeing the big picture and our specific actions as just one part of a major
plan or purpose. For example, someone trying to eat healthily should focus on
the ultimate goal and how each individual decision about what to eat
contributes (or detracts) from that goal.
· Abstract reasoning. This means trying to avoid considering the specific details of
the situation at hand in favour of thinking about how actions fit into an
overall framework - being philosophical. Someone trying to add more
self-control to their exercise regime might try to think less about the details
of the exercise, and instead focus on an abstract vision of the ideal physical
self, or how exercise provides a time to re-connect mind and body.
· High-level
categorisation. This means thinking about
high-level concepts rather than specific instances. Any long-term project,
whether in business, academia or elsewhere can easily get bogged down by
focusing too much on the minutiae of everyday processes and forgetting the
ultimate goal. Categorising tasks or project stages conceptually may help an
individual or group maintain their focus and achieve greater self-discipline.
These are just some examples of specific instances, but with a
little creativity the same principles can be applied to many situations in
which self-control is required. Ultimately these three ways of thinking are
different ways of saying much the same thing: avoid thinking locally and
specifically and practice thinking globally, objectively and abstractly, and
increased self-control should follow.
No comments:
Post a Comment